Image
  Why Should We Be Concerned About the China-India Border Conflict Long-standing border tensions risk dangerous escalation as rivalry between these nuclear powers heats up. The conflict between Chinese and Indian troops over the two nations' 2,100-mile-long contentious border, known as the Line of Actual Control (LAC), in December 2022, demonstrates a concerning "one step forward, two steps back" tendency. This brawl was the bloodiest in the Galwan Valley since 2020, when violence killed 20 Indian and at least four Chinese soldiers. Although these skirmishes are frequently followed by talks and other measures to alleviate tensions, both parties have militarised their border policy and show no signs of relenting. And the border situation remains tight, with Beijing and New Delhi reinforcing their postures on either side of the LAC, raising the prospect of an escalation between the two nuclear-armed countries. On June 12, 2009, Indian soldiers are spotted in Tawang Va

Puberty can fix stress after adversity early in life

 

A researcher puts stickers on a lazy Susan under some plastic cups, then offers a whirl. A preschooler must find hidden stickers when the spinning ends. Some kids remember where the stickers are, but some have to search each cup.

The game measures working memory, one of the group of behavioral abilities known as executive control that can be compromised in early-life trauma-facing adolescents.

Adversity wreaks havoc, and from there you have a mechanism that reacts differently, says Megan Gunnar, a developmental psychobiologist at Minnesota University in Minneapolis who spent two decades researching the effects of early-life adversity in adopted kids. This work focuses on acute hardship, such as being orphaned, rather than regular struggles, which may teach valuable resilience.

An infancy marked by deprivation, neglect or violence may also change the neuroendocrine system governing how the body reacts to stress. Stress response issues will put children on a road to developmental challenges along with increased risk of adhd, diabetes, and a host of other health concerns.

Yet new reports indicate that a daunting future might not be necessary. As Gunnar and others have demonstrated, disrupted stress responses can revert to normal during puberty, making it easier to remove imbalances produced by early trauma. Research is encouraging a new understanding of puberty as an opportunity — a chance for adults with a weak beginning to change their physiological responses to stress.

A sense of safety

When the brain perceives a threat — even a fleeting one, such as a painful test or a high-stakes competition — levels of hormone adrenaline shot up , causing the "fight-or - flight" reflex. Breathing and pace ascend. Palms hot. See and sharpen senses. The brain sends chemical messengers to activate adrenal glands near the kidneys to release cortisol.

Cortisol transfers sugars to blood for quick energy. In a fight-or - flight case, the hormone often delays metabolism, immune responses, development and other functions considered non-essential.

The fight-or - flight response stops when the danger disappears, at least in a person whose stress response functions as it should. Levels of adrenaline and cortisol drops, pulse rate slows, and other processes continue business as normal.

When Gunnar began her doctoral study in the 1970s , researchers already figured out main players in the stress response. Involved neuroendocrine impulses form the HPA axis, short for hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal. When rodents and monkeys face early difficulty, the HPA axis is knocked off-kilter. When techniques were possible to quantify cortisol from saliva samples — rather than extracting blood or urine — Gunnar set out to research how the HPA axis affects human brain and conduct.

From mid-1980s studies with newborn babies, Gunnar found that having a stable parent relationship is crucial for a healthy neuroendocrine system and helps babies cope with challenging conditions such as getting immunizations. You can go to the hospital as a infant and have a huge shot in one leg and the other leg, and you're tearing off your back ... but it doesn't take off, says Gunnar. But if babies are separated from their parents for even a few minutes, their HPA axis rockets up like a missile.

Gunnar wonders what happens if the long-term feeling of protection is broken. She attempted to research neglected and deprived children, but distinguishing the consequences of early deprivation from later problems is not straightforward. "The way you start life seems to continue," says Gunnar. A US-published paper last November. Centers on Disease Control and Prevention refers to the long-term effects of smoking, drinking excessively, dropping out of high school and having heart disease and a host of other health problems relative to their peers.

A mid-1990s trip put Gunnar on a new direction to address the troublesome study issue. She went to an orphanage in eastern Romania with a study team, where children were living in overcrowded, barbaric circumstances. You step into these wards, and immediately you're mobbed by kids shouting 'Mom, Mom, Mom' ... stretching their arms to get hold, says Gunnar, who at the time had two school-age sons. It was bad. I just tried to get them home.

What she carried back to Minnesota was a series of small vials, each holding a saliva sample from a 2- or 3-year-old orphan. To her dismay, cortisol levels in kids — the end result of the neuroendocrine cascade — were smaller than the normal people. The study provided a window on long-term parental neglect effects on stress responses.

Adoptee struggles

To recognise the impact of early adversity, Gunnar wanted kids who began out in misery but migrated to stable, nurturing environments after infancy. Such kids will be the perfect model for all early adversity animal experiments, she thought. It dawned that this community exists: orphans adopted.

Gunnar shared her proposal with employees of the Minnesota Human Services Department's adoption team. With support and funds from the agency, she surveyed Minnesota parents who had adopted children abroad in the 1990s, encouraging families to enter a university registry and engage in research.

 

Many research parents had found early that their adoptive children had behavioural issues. And when the kids came to the university lab for problem-solving and sorting exercises, including the lazy Susan challenge and the successful marshmallow test of delayed gratification, the kids struggled with treatment and self-regulation.

Like Romanian orphans, these kids had lower amounts of cortisol than children who had no behavioural issues. Gunnar speculates that in the face of prolonged suffering, which has the potential to promote excessively high levels of cortisol, a poor stress response — that is, generating less cortisol — could be the way nature protects the brain and body.

Teen-time shift

Adopted children who grew up with early-life abuse (gray curve) before puberty had blunted stress responses compared to children with birth parents (blue curve). Before, after and after a challenging mission, the adopted kids showed regular cortisol levels. Saliva was extracted 20 minutes before the mission, 5, 20, 40, 60 and 80 minutes after the assignment. Researchers translated data to a logarithmic scale, displaying negative figures. Cortisol amounts range between 0 and 1 microgram per deciliter.

Studying adoptees over time, she noticed that care-probleming preschoolers with low cortisol often reached kindergarten. A blunted stress response continued in middle childhood, even after an average seven or eight years in a safe care home.

 

That was disheartening, says Russell Romeo, Barnard College psychobiologist in New York City. We 'd always figured they would start recalibrating their stress reactivity once they get out of stressful circumstances.

 

But Romeo 's study in the mid-2000s gave Gunnar cause to believe she wanted to look deeper into the direction of children's lives.


High time for change

Romeo examined rats to see how depression influences teenage and adult brains differently. In one series of tests, he exposed adult rats and prepubescent rats to acute stress — 30 minutes stuck inside a wire mesh jar — and reported their corticosterone levels (cortisol rat version) before, during and after confinement. Both groups developed similar hormone spikes when stressed, but levels in juvenile rats took much longer to normalize.

When Romeo studied how animals responded to long stress periods—30 minutes of seven-day restriction per day — the behavior was different. Since freeing the animals from the chains, tension hormones spiked higher in young rats than in adults. Yet rats near puberty returned to baseline quicker than older males. Taken together, research by Romeo proposed neuroendocrine stress responses were formed during adolescence to appear differently in adulthood.

In earlier studies , researchers at McGill University in Montreal found that transferring teenage rats to enriched environments — larger cages with more toys and cagemates — could reset stress mechanisms that early-life deprivation had thrown out of whack.

 

Ses results inspired Gunnar. Maybe I'd look into puberty, she figured. It may be recalibrating time.

Closer to normal

During traumatic behavior (such as speaking), saliva cortisol levels increased briefly and returned to usual among those children who stayed with their biological parents. Children adopted from childhood in an orphanage (an early-life trauma) had blunted cortisol responses at stages 1 to 2 of puberty. Yet at the rear end of puberty, stages 4 and 5, accepted standardized infant stress responses.

Thus, her team welcomed 280 7- to 14-year-olds—122 youngsters adopted from organizations and 158 from social comparable maternal families — to the laboratory to perform two challenging tasks. One was demanding theoretical arithmetic. For the second assignment, each pupil practiced a five-minute speech announcing a new student class. Children were told their voice, presented in front of a video camera and mirror, would be judged. Some kids talked enthusiastically, others seemed anxious. We had one break into laughter, Gunnar says. So we're not torturing them. If we find they 're too anxious, we 're making them leave.

Before and during each child's speech and math activities, researchers gathered saliva samples to test cortisol levels. The pubertal status of the participants was measured on a 1-to-5 scale: stage 1 meant no visible body modifications and stage 5 meant full sexual maturation.

Both early puberty kids (stages 1–2), adoptive kids had blunted cortisol levels before and after the activities similar to kids living with their biological parents. This finding validated Gunnar 's previous pre-school-age foreign adoption study. Cortisol trends in late puberty group (stages 4–5) appeared identical for adopted and unadopted adolescents.

In order to confirm HPA recalibration, instead of simply contrasting across age groups, Gunnar and colleagues took participants for the same assessments one and two years later, for a total of three annual sessions.

Matthew Duggan, a Long Beach, California psychiatrist working in childhood and puberty, is inspired by results and feels they could relate to a wide variety of children who have difficulty controlling their feelings and communicating with others when parents neglected or overlooked them early in life. There may be a chance ... where things may change, Duggan says. And we have some evidence to show that this is a probability at biological stage. That's very optimistic for me.

Duggan suggests Gunnar 's study would have been even more beneficial if it had measured the change in behavior of participants — for example, by questioning parents , teachers, or teenagers themselves.

How can puberty combine better treatment and help to reshape neuroendocrine stress responses? Romeo speculates that stems from the fact that the hypothalamus and other brain areas, such as the prefrontal cortex, that regulate our stress reactions are among the regions that rewire and establish associations throughout adolescence.

It's impossible to tell whether these changes in the stress response will actually benefit a young person, Gunnar says. Mental wellbeing and endurance arise from an ever-changing mix of genes and social experiences — some of which early bring the body awry. But puberty may probably remove any harm, her research reveals. Gunnar and others intend to discover more of the fundamental reboot biology.

 


Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Iran is preparing to swiftly increase oil production if US sanctions are lifted.

Ancient arms race sharpened our immune system, which still left us weak

Hackers can steal BRAIN WAVES